Javascript required
Skip to content Skip to sidebar Skip to footer

Why Is There Male Babies More Than Females Sids

Sleep. 2010 Aug 1; 33(8): 1055–1060.

Sleeping Like a Baby—Does Gender Influence Infant Arousability?

Heidi L. Richardson

The Ritchie Center, Monash Institute of Medical Research, Monash University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia

Adrian M. Walker

The Ritchie Heart, Monash Institute of Medical Inquiry, Monash University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia

Rosemary Due south.C. Horne

The Ritchie Centre, Monash Institute of Medical Research, Monash Academy, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia

Received 2009 Aug; Revised 2009 October; Accepted 2009 Oct.

Abstract

Introduction:

Victims of the sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) may take preexisting abnormalities in their arousal pathways, inhibiting the progression of subcortical activation (SCA) to full cortical arousal (CA). Approximately sixty% of SIDS victims are male, and information technology has been suggested that male infants take delayed cortical maturation compared to females. We hypothesized that CA frequency would be lower and CA threshold would exist higher in male infants during both active (AS) and repose (QS) sleep.

Methods:

fifty salubrious term infants (21 male, 29 female) were studied with daytime polysomnography at 2–4 weeks and 2–3 months after nativity. Arousal from slumber was induced using a pulsatile air-jet to the nostrils at increasing pressures.

Results:

At 2–iv weeks, arousability from AS was similar in males and females, however during QS, male person infants required a lower stimulus to induce SCA and CA. This gender difference in arousal threshold was not observed at 2–3 months. CA frequencies were like between genders during both sleep states at both ages, though overall, CA was more frequent in Equally than in QS.

Conclusions:

This study demonstrated that at 2–4 weeks, male person infants were easier to agitate than female infants during QS. There were no significant furnishings of gender on full arousability or SCA and CA frequencies at 2–3 months, the historic period of top SIDS incidence. Thus, although male infants are at greater run a risk of SIDS than female infants, this divergence is unlikely to be associated with gender differences in CA threshold or frequency.

Commendation:

Richardson HL; Walker AM; Horne RSC. Sleeping like a baby—does gender influence babe arousability? SLEEP 2010;33(8):1055-1060.

Keywords: Baby, arousal, SIDS, slumber, gender

SUDDEN Babe DEATH SYNDROME (SIDS) REMAINS A LEADING Crusade OF INFANT MORTALITY IN Developed COUNTRIES, WITH AN INCREASING NUMBER of deaths attributed to SIDS over recent years.1,2 Although the exact causal mechanisms remain an enigma, it is commonly idea that the terminal event of SIDS involves failed or impaired arousal from sleep in response to a life-threatening cardiorespiratory claiming.3–five In support of this proposal, postmortem examinations of SIDS victims have revealed pathological show that prolonged or intermittent hypoxia may take preceded decease.vi,7 In further support of the failed arousal hypothesis, other studies accept identified abnormalities in the brainstem of SIDS victims. These include disturbances in cholinergic and serotonergic (5-HT) systems,8,9 also as structural abnormalities in medullary nuclei associated with the central control of respiratory and circulatory defensive responses and slumber/wake states.10,eleven

The infant arousal response is considered to be hierarchical in nature, consisting of subcortical or "brainstem" responses such as centre rate, respiratory, and behavioral changes that occur without agonizing sleep integrity, and those which progress to cortical activation with or without a total enkindling.12,13 Physiological testify suggests that despite actualization normal and apparently well prior to their deaths, SIDS victims may have had a functional deficit in the arousal process that inhibited the progression of subcortical activation (SCA) to total cortical arousal (CA).14,fifteen Furthermore, as previously shown in good for you infants, exposure to the major risk factors for SIDS, prone sleeping and maternal smoking, modifies the progression of stimulus-induced arousal processes from SCA to CA.16,17 Peradventure, an inhibited progression of SCA to CA may provide a marker of an infant at increased likelihood of succumbing to SIDS, envisaged by Filiano and Kinney in their triple risk model for SIDS as the "vulnerable infant."18

Approximately 60% of SIDS victims are male, and this increased prevalence of SIDS in male person infants has been demonstrated consistently throughout the globe.ii,19–22 Inherent gender differences accept been identified in the medullae of SIDS victims, including increased apoptotic neuronal prison cell death and decreased 5-HT1A receptor binding in male infants that may lead to altered arousal pathways and increased vulnerability to SIDS.eight,11 Maturation of the central nervous organisation and cortical office occurs more slowly in male person infants23; but it remains uncertain whether there are specific gender differences in the arousal process. We hypothesized that CA responses would exist impaired in male infants when compared with female infants, contributing to the increased risk of SIDS in males. To test this hypothesis we evaluated induced arousal responses from active (Every bit) and quiet (QS) slumber in male and female person infants, with a focus on the progression from SCA to CA.

METHODS

Ethical approval for this project was obtained from the Southern Wellness and Monash University Homo Research Ethics Committees. Participation was voluntary, with no budgetary incentive provided. Written informed parental consent was obtained prior to study offset.

Participants

A total of fifty salubrious infants were studied at 2–4 weeks and two–3 months postnatal age. As shown past the demographic information in Tabular array 1, all male (n = 21) and female (n = 29) infants were born at term and were of similar birth weights. Subject area inclusion criteria controlled for known SIDS risk factors in that none of the mothers smoked during pregnancy, infants were predominantly chest fed and were all routinely placed supine to sleep at domicile.

Tabular array one

Infant demographics (hateful ± SEM)

Birth two–4 weeks 2–3 months
Infants, due north Male 21 xx 17
Female 29 28 29

Historic period Male 39.8 ± 0.2 wk 22 ± one d 79 ± 2 d
Female 39.6 ± 0.2 wk 24 ± ane d 78 ± 2 d

Weight, g Male 3505 ± 106 3996 ± 136 6027 ± 200
Female 3465 ± 76 3935 ± 97 5213 ± 154

Study Protocol

Daytime polysomnography was performed in a sleep laboratory where low-cal and dissonance were minimal and ambient temperature was maintained at approximately 23°C. During a routine morning feed, leads were attached for the measurement of electroencephalogram (EEG), left and right electrooculogram, mental-submental electromyogram (EMG), electrocardiogram, abdominal peel temperature (YSI 400 Serial Thermistor, Mallinckrodt, Melbourne, VIC, Australia), thoracic and abdominal breathing movements (Piezo-electric sensors, Resp-ez, EPM Systems, VA, U.s.a.), and blood oxygen saturation (SpO2; BIOX 3700e pulse oximeter, Ohmeda, CO, USA). Signals were recorded with a 16-channel polygraph (Model 78A, Grass Musical instrument Co., Quincy, MA, USA) at a sampling rate of 500 Hz.

Infants were placed supine in a bassinet to sleep. During both AS and QS (scored according to standard criteria24), a series of trigeminal stimuli were used to assess infant arousability as previously described.25 Briefly, a pulsatile jet of air (pulse frequency of 3 Hz, for 5s duration) was delivered via a manus-held cannula, to the left and right nostrils alternately. Stimulus driving pressures ranged betwixt 25 to 900 cm H2O (the pressure practical to the infant was much less, existence a maximum of ane.4 cm H2O); beginning low (50 cm H2O), pressures were generally increased by 100 cm H2O betwixt consecutive stimuli, until an arousal response was observed and pressures were and so subsequently reduced.

Data Assay

Responses to each stimulus were classified as either non-arousal, SCA, or CA, in accordance with the standard definitions for infant arousal from sleep.thirteen With the preceding 10s as the physiological baseline, SCA was identified by the presence of ≥ two of the following criteria within 7s of initiating the air-jet; a gross torso motility, a heart rate (HR) increase ≥ 10%, an increased EMG amplitude (AS only), and a change in respiratory frequency or amplitude (QS only). CA was visually scored using the aforementioned criteria, with the add-on of a shift in background EEG frequency (EEG desynchronization) for ≥ 3s.

The percentages of stimuli which resulted in a non-arousal, as well as the mean driving pressure required to arm-twist an arousal response (either SCA or CA), were calculated to assess total arousability. In a more detailed investigation of the arousal procedure, SCA and CA were then analyzed as separate components of the arousal response. SCA and CA were too analyzed equally percentages of total arousal responses, excluding the non-arousing tests, to command for any differences in total arousability. In addition, as an indication of the magnitude of these responses, the tiptop Hour change recorded during both forms of arousal, and the duration of EEG desynchronization associated with CA were likewise measured. Individual infants were represented by a single average value of each parameter for each slumber land (AS and QS) at each written report age. To determine the effects of sleep land and gender on baseline (HR and respiratory frequency) and arousal parameters, data were first tested for normality and equal variance (Sigmastat three.0, Systat Software Inc., Richmond, CA, USA), then compared using 2-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Student Newman-Keuls post hoc analysis. Statistical significance was considered as P < 0.05 and data are presented equally hateful ± standard mistake of the hateful (SEM).

RESULTS

Infant numbers, ages and weights for each written report are displayed in Table one. Due to technical complications or infants beingness unavailable for either one of the ii study ages, six infants were not studied longitudinally; information were missing for 1 male and 1 female at 2–4 weeks, and for 4 male infants at two–three months. Demographics were like for male person and female person infants, though male infants were heavier at 2–3 months (P < 0.05). Equally shown past Table two, the full number of air-jet stimuli was like betwixt genders and sleep states, with the one exception beingness fewer tests performed during AS in female infants at 2–3 months (P < 0.05).

Table 2

Arousal responses to air-jet stimuli in male and female infants (data represented every bit hateful ± SEM)

2–4 weeks
2–iii months
Active Slumber Tranquillity Slumber Active Sleep Quiet Sleep
Full Stimuli, north Male 21 ± two 18 ± ii xix ± 3 20 ± 2
Female 21 ± ii nineteen ± 2 12 ± 1* # 19 ± 2

Arousal Threshold, cm H2O Male 206 ± 22 242 ± 27* 136 ± 21 294 ± 39 #
Female person 182 ± fifteen 357 ± 37 # 103 ± 12 336 ± 35 #

Non-arousals, % Male 59 ± 2 61 ± 4 54 ± 3 63 ± iii
Female 59 ± 2 65 ± 3 48 ± 3 65 ± 3 #

SCA, % Male 26 ± 2 34 ± four 33 ± 4 31 ± iii
Female 28 ± ii 29 ± iii 37 ± 4 27 ± 3

CA, % Male fifteen ± 2 5 ± 1 # 12 ± 3 six ± 1
Female person 13 ± 2 six ± 1 # 15 ± 2 seven ± 2 #

Total Arousability

Responses to air-jet

The % of air-jet stimuli which failed to induce arousal from slumber were not different between males and females or between AS and QS at two–4 weeks. When infants were 2–3 months, there were no significant effects of gender on the % of not-arousing air-jet tests. QS was associated with more frequent non-arousal responses (i.eastward. decreased arousability) than Equally (ANOVA, P < 0.001); this difference was identified as significant just in the female infants after post hoc assay (P < 0.001, Table ii).

Stimulus intensity

At both ages studied, the mean driving pressures which led to an arousal from slumber (either SCA or CA thresholds) were significantly higher during QS (P < 0.001), existence by and large more than than doubled (Table 2); however in the male infants at 2–four weeks, there was no QS-related increase. Consequently, when compared with the female person infants, the QS arousal threshold was lower in males (P < 0.01).

SCA and CA

Arousal frequency

SCA and CA frequencies were analyzed both equally percentages of total stimuli and full arousal responses; the results were the same for both methods, hence merely the percentages of stimuli which resulted in SCA and CA are displayed in Table ii. Neither slumber country nor gender had whatsoever pregnant furnishings on the percentage of SCA at either age. Similarly, CA frequencies were not different betwixt genders. However, a decreased frequency of CA was associated with QS compared with As at both study ages (P < 0.001), this merely failed to achieve significance in the male infants at 2–3 months (P = 0.06).

Stimulus intensity

The air-jet driving pressures which induced SCA and CA are displayed in Figure ane. For both SCA and CA, arousing stimulus pressures did not differ between genders; however a significant gender-sleep state interaction was observed for CA thresholds at 2–4 weeks whereby male infants were more hands aroused (i.east. at lower pressures) from QS than females (P < 0.05). Like trends for the males to exhibit lower arousal thresholds were as well evident during QS, for SCA at 2–4 weeks and CA at 2–3 months.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is aasm.33.8.1055a.jpg

Mean driving pressures of air-jet stimuli which induced subcortical activation (SCA) and cortical arousals (CA) at (A) 2–4 weeks and (B) 2–3 months, in AS (solid bars) and QS (striped confined), for male (black bars) and female (grey bars) infants. *P < 0.05, male vs. female person, #P < 0.05, As vs. QS.

At both study ages, for both SCA and CA, infants required higher stimulus pressures to arouse during QS than Every bit (ANOVA, P < 0.001). Withal at 2–iv weeks, this sleep state effect was but significant in the female infants, with males exhibiting similar arousal thresholds during the two slumber states (P > 0.05, Figure iA). At two–iii months, SCA-inducing pressures were significantly lower during Equally than QS for both genders (P < 0.01, Effigy 1B). This slumber-country deviation was too observed in the air-jet pressures which induced CA, though significance in post hoc analysis was confined to the female infants (P < 0.05).

Magnitude of Responses

Eye rate

Effigy 2 shows baseline Hour values and the top Hr changes observed during SCA and CA. There were no effects of gender or sleep land on baseline Hour (Figure iiA) or peak Hr change (Figure twoB and C) during CA at either postnatal age. At 2–4 weeks, Hr changes associated with SCA were reduced during QS when compared with Every bit in female infants just (P < 0.05, Figure iiB), and as well tended to be reduced when compared with male person infants in QS. A similar trend for smaller HR responses in the female infants was observed at 2-three months, although this was meaning for SCA just during QS (P < 0.05, Figure 2C).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is aasm.33.8.1055b.jpg

(A) Baseline 60 minutes information and (B) % 60 minutes change during arousal from slumber at 2–4 weeks and (C) at 2–3 months. Data are shown for male (black bars) and female (grey bars) infants, during both Every bit (solid bars) and QS (striped bars). *P < 0.05, male person vs. female, #P < 0.05, AS vs. QS.

EEG desynchronization

The hateful durations of EEG desynchronization associated with CA were 11.one ± 0.8s at two–4 weeks and 9.4 ± 0.8s at 2–iii months, and were non different between sleep states or betwixt male person and female infants.

DISCUSSION

Increased vulnerability of the male gender to SIDS and numerous other morbidities has been well-documented,26–30 with this general "male person disadvantage" perhaps resulting from physiological gender differences during development.31 Given the widely proposed relationship between impaired arousability and SIDS,3–5,fourteen nosotros aimed to comprehensively investigate the effects of gender on arousal from sleep processes, accounting for the progression of SCA to full CA. We demonstrated that at 2–4 weeks postnatal historic period, male infants were more hands aroused from QS than female infants, i.due east. a smaller stimulus was required to induce both SCA and CA. However, this gender deviation in arousal threshold was not observed at ii–iii months postnatal age, the age of peak SIDS risk.

Our finding that female infants appeared to slumber more soundly than male infants is consistent with previous reports of increased sleep disruption in male versus female infants; specifically, the maternal perception of their baby'southward sleep patterns take included more problematic crying and increased night awakenings in male infants.32,33 Similarly, another study observed shorter sleep periods at night in male person infants and suggested that, throughout the start 6 months of age, the normal evolution of sleeping rhythms is delayed in males when compared with female infants.34 In addition to the evolution of circadian rhythms and slumber architecture, EEG patterns too undergo dramatic maturational changes throughout the outset months of life. These EEG changes are particularly prominent during QS and reverberate the occurrence of precise synaptogenesis and the formation of thalamocortical and intracortical connections important in sensory input and arousal.35 In the present study, the gender divergence in arousal thresholds was only observed during QS, not in As. Thus, the male infants exhibited an increased level of arousability in QS that was comparable to that in AS, not reduced arousability as exhibited by female infants. In addition, our cardiovascular analysis revealed that 60 minutes acceleration during arousals tended to be larger in the males compared with their female counterparts. Similarly, a previous study by Nagy et al. reported increased HR reactivity to the Moro (startle) reflex in males.34 It is possible that male infants agitate more readily to external stimuli that are suppressed in female person infants. It may be that thalamic and extrathalamic processes which serve to filter coincident stimuli may exist immature or underdeveloped when compared with female person infants at the same age. The view that primal nervous arrangement maturation is delayed in male infants is further supported by our finding that the male-related decrease in induced arousal thresholds for both SCA and CA during QS was credible at the younger historic period of 2–4 weeks, but non observed at 2–3 months.

In contrast to our current findings, a previous study in our laboratory identified no significant differences between male and female infants when arousal thresholds were compared throughout the start half dozen months of life.36 We accredit the disparity in results betwixt these two studies to the refinement of standard criteria for defining arousal responses.13 Chiefly, by distinguishing betwixt SCA and CA, the present written report represents a more than sensitive approach than the earlier study which examined full arousability.

Impairment of arousal processes is considered to be an of import cistron in the pathogenesis of SIDS and the greater risk of male infants succumbing to SIDS has been well documented over the years.2,19–21 Despite observing gender effects on the force of the stimulus required to arm-twist arousal responses, our examination of the relative frequencies of SCA and CA revealed no significant differences between male person and female infants, either in relation to sleep state or postnatal historic period. This finding was not predicted by our starting hypothesis that male person infants have inhibited CA responses when compared to female infants. We reasoned that if arousal progression from SCA to CA was compromised, there would be a decreased CA frequency and an increased SCA frequency, like to the response patterns documented in SIDS victims preceding their deaths.14 Rather, in the present study SCA and CA frequencies were not dissimilar betwixt male and female person infants. Furthermore, the duration of EEG desynchronization associated with CA was also similar. Together, these findings suggest that once initiated, the arousal procedure is unchanged by gender. Thus, while the gender-related arousal threshold differences may be significant, there are no gender differences in arousal progression from SCA to CA that could increase the vulnerability of male person infants to SIDS.

In the present study, approximately threescore% of air-jet stimuli failed to induce an arousal response (Tabular array two). Rather than a physiological phenomenon, this high pct of non-arousal responses is a reflection of the stepwise nature of the stimulus protocol. In each series of tests, air-jet stimulation commenced at a low driving pressure of 50 cm HiiO and was gradually increased in intensity until an arousal response was observed. Subsequent stimuli pressures were adjusted to surround infants' arousal thresholds, thus it was expected that at to the lowest degree fifty% of stimuli would upshot in a non-arousal. It is besides important to notation that when these non-arousing tests were excluded from data analyses, the effects of gender and slumber state on SCA and CA frequencies remained identical.

The absenteeism of blunted arousal processes in the healthy male infants of this present report suggests that the gender-related vulnerability to SIDS is not a preexisting status; however, we have previously demonstrated that infant arousal processes tin can be modified past external factors such as the major take a chance factors for SIDS—prone sleeping and maternal smoking.16,17 Although the increased incidence of SIDS observed in male infants may not be due to a preexisting vulnerability, it is maybe a secondary consequence of the infants' environs or state of affairs, i.e. the gender outcome may exist a upshot of an "exogenous stress" or trigger, as postulated by the triple risk model for SIDS.18 An earlier written report in the Netherlands establish that more males were beingness placed prone to sleep than females, though authors offered no explanation for the phenomenon.37 Equally previously stated, male infants are perceived to be more unsettled than female infants32,33 and studies take shown that excessive crying is ane of the primary reasons parents and other caregivers report for placing infants to slumber in the decumbent position.38 Consequently, a higher prevalence of prone sleeping as a settling technique may contribute to the increased SIDS rates in males.19,39

Our study also investigated the effects of sleep state on arousal progression, as AS and QS are physiologically distinct, and in relation to SIDS adventure, researchers have withal to identify in which slumber state infants may be more than vulnerable. Respiratory and cardiovascular irregularities occur more frequently during AS, perhaps necessitating an adequate arousal response for maintaining homeostasis. On the other hand, arousability is already depressed during QS,25 which may pose an increased risk, despite the cardiorespiratory stability of this state. By considering both SCA and CA, the present study adds to previous observations of the more than arousable nature of infants during AS; overall, non-arousals occurred less frequently and CA more than frequently in AS than in QS. The frequencies of induced SCA responses were consistent betwixt both Equally and QS, providing support for the single previous report which found no sleep state differences on spontaneous SCA until infants were five–half dozen months of age.40

CONCLUSION

At two–4 weeks, male infants are more easily aroused than female infants, though we observed no significant effects of gender on total arousability or SCA and CA frequencies at 2–three months, the age of meridian SIDS incidence. Thus, although male person infants are at greater risk of SIDS than female infants, it is unlikely that this increased vulnerability is caused past a pre-existing inhibition of CA processes; however we speculate that male infants may be more susceptible to being placed in at-risk situations.

DISCLOSURE STATEMENT

This was not an industry supported study. The authors have indicated no financial conflicts of involvement.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to thank all the parents and infants who participated in this study.

Abbreviations

SIDS sudden infant expiry syndrome
Equally active sleep
QS quiet sleep
SCA subcortical activation
CA cortical arousal

REFERENCES

i. Kung H, Hoyert DL, Xu J, Murphy SL. Deaths: Final information for 2005. Natl Vital Stat Rep. 2008;56(ten) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

2. Australian Agency of Statistics. 2007 Causes of Death. Canberra, Australia: 2009. [Google Scholar]

3. Phillipson EA, Sullivan CE. Arousal: the forgotten response to respiratory stimuli. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1978;118:807–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

4. Harper RM. The cerebral regulation of cardiovascular and respiratory functions. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 1996;iii:13–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

5. Kahn A, Sawaguchi T, Sawaguchi A, et al. Sudden infant deaths: from epidemiology to physiology. Forensic Sci Int. 2002;130S:S8–S20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

6. Opdal SH, Rognum TO, Vege A, Saugstad OL. Hypoxanthine levels in vitreous humor: a study of influencing factors in sudden baby death syndrome. Pediatr Res. 1998;44:192–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

7. Jones KL, Krous HF, Nadeau J, Blackbourne B, Zielke HR, Gozal D. Vascular endothelial growth factor in the cerebrospinal fluid of infants who died of sudden infant death syndrome: testify for antecedent hypoxia. Pediatrics. 2003;111:358–63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

eight. Paterson DS, Trachtenberg FL, Thompson EG, et al. Multiple serotonergic brainstem abnormalities in sudden baby death syndrome. JAMA. 2006;296:2124–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Kinney HC, Filiano JJ, Sleeper LA, Mandell F, Valdes-Dapena M, White WF. Decreased muscarinic receptor binding in the arcuate nucleus in sudden infant decease syndrome. Science. 1995;269:1446–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Lavezzi AM, Ottaviani G, Mauri Chiliad, Matturri L. Hypoplasia of the arcuate nucleus and maternal smoking during pregnancy in sudden unexplained perinatal and babe death. Neuropathology. 2004;24:284–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

11. Machaalani R, Waters KA. Neuronal jail cell expiry in the sudden infant death syndrome brainstem and associations with gamble factors. Brain. 2008;131:218–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

12. McNamara F, Wulbrand H, Thach BT. Characteristics of the babe arousal response. J Appl Physiol. 1998;83:219–28. [Google Scholar]

xiii. The International Paediatric Work Group on Arousals. The scoring of arousals in healthy term infants (between the ages of ane and 6 months) J Slumber Res. 2005;14:37–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

14. Kato I, Franco P, Groswasser J, et al. Incomplete arousal processes in infants who were victims of sudden death. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2003;168:1298–303. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Harper RM. Impaired arousals and sudden babe death syndrome. Am J Respir Crit Intendance Med. 2003;168:1262–three. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Richardson HL, Walker AM, Horne RSC. Sleep position alters arousal processes maximally at the high-take chances age for sudden infant death syndrome. J Sleep Res. 2008;17:450–vii. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

17. Richardson HL, Walker AM, Horne RSC. Maternal smoking impairs arousal pathways in sleeping infants. Sleep. 2008;32:515–21. [PMC costless article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

18. Filiano JJ, Kinney HC. A perspective on neuropathologic findings in victims of the sudden infant death syndrome. Biol Neonate. 1994;65:194–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

xix. Carpenter RG, Irgens LM, England PD, et al. Sudden unexplained infant death in 20 regions in Europe: case control study. Lancet. 2004;363:185–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. Blair PS, Sidebotham P, Berry PJ, Evans M, Fleming PJ. Major epidemiological changes in Sudden Babe Decease Syndrome: a 20-twelvemonth population-based study in the United kingdom. Lancet. 2006;367:314–ix. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

21. Mage DT, Donner M. Female resistance to hypoxia: does it explain the sex difference in mortality rates? J Womens Health. 2006;15:786–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Moon RY, Horne RSC, Hauck FR. Sudden infant death syndrome. Lancet. 2007;370:1578–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

23. Thordstein M, Lofgren North, Flisberg A, Lindecrantz K, Kjellmer I. Sexual practice differences in electrocortical activeness in human neonates. Neuroreport. 2006;17:1165–viii. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Curzi-Dascalova L, Mirmiran Thousand. Manual of methods for recording and analyzing sleep-wakefulness states in preterm and full-term infant. Paris: Les Editions INSERM; 1996. [Google Scholar]

25. Parslow PM, Harding R, Cranage SM, Adamson TM, Horne RSC. Arousal responses to somatosensory and mild hypoxic stimuli are depressed during quiet sleep in salubrious term infants. Sleep. 2003;26:739–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. McGregor JA, Leff M, Orleans Thousand, Baron A. Fetal gender differences in preterm birth: findings in a N American cohort. Am J Perinatol. 1992;9:43–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

27. Stevenson DK, Verter J, Fanaroff AA, et al. Sex activity differences in outcomes of very depression birthweight infants: the newborn male person disadvantage. Arch Dis Kid Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2000;83:F182–5. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

28. Vohr BH, Wright LL, Dusick AM, et al. Neurodevelopmental and functional outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants in the National Found of Child Wellness and Human Development Neonatal Research Network, 1993-1994. Pediatrics. 2000;105:1216–26. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

29. Banci M, Saccucci P, Dofcaci A, et al. Birth weight and coronary artery affliction. The upshot of gender and diabetes. Int J Biol Sci. 2009;v:244–8. [PMC complimentary article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

xxx. Dasgupta K, O'Loughlin J, Chen Southward, et al. Emergence of sexual practice differences in prevalence of high systolic claret pressure: Analysis of a longitudinal boyish cohort. Apportionment. 2006;114:2663–lxx. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

31. Elsmen E, Pupp IH, Hellstrom-Westas L. Preterm male infants need more than initial respiratory and circulatory back up than female infants. Acta Paediatr. 2004;93:529–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

32. van der Wal MF, van den Boom DC, Pauw-Plomp H, de Jonge GA. Mothers' reports of babe crying and soothing in a multicultural population. Arch Dis Kid. 1998;79:312–seven. [PMC costless commodity] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

33. Anuntaseree W, Mo-suwan L, Vasiknanonte P, Kuasirikul Southward, Ma-a-lee A, Choprapawan C. Night waking in Thai infants at iii months of historic period: association betwixt parental practices and babe sleep. Sleep Med. 2008;nine:564–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

34. Nagy E, Loveland KA, Orvos H, Molnar P. Gender-related physiologic differences in human neonates and the greater vulnerability of males to developmental encephalon disorders. J Gend Specif Med. 2001;4:41–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

35. Heraghty JL, Hilliard TN, Henderson AJ, Fleming PJ. The physiology of sleep in infants. Arch Dis Child. 2008;93:982–v. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

36. Horne RSC, Parslow PM, Ferens D, Watts A-Thou, Adamson TM. Comparing of evoked arousability in chest and formula fed infants. Arch Dis Child. 2004;89:22–5. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

37. de Jonge GA, Burgmeijer RJF, Engelberts AC, Hoogenboezem J, Kostense PJ, Sprij AJ. Sleeping position for infants and cot expiry in the netherlands 1985-91. Arch Dis Child. 1993;69:660–3. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

38. Willinger MP, Ko C-WP, Hoffman HJMA, Kessler RCP, Corwin MJMD. Factors associated with caregivers' choice of infant sleep position, 1994-1998: the National Infant Sleep Position Study. JAMA. 2000;283:2135–42. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

39. 50'Hoir MP, Engelberts AC, van Well GTJ, et al. Risk and preventive factors for cot death in The Netherlands, a low-incidence country. Eur J Pediatr. 1998;157:681–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

40. Montemitro E, Franco P, Scaillet South, et al. Maturation of spontaneous arousals in good for you infants. Sleep. 2008;31:47–54. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Why Is There Male Babies More Than Females Sids

Source: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2910535/#:~:text=Approximately%2060%25%20of%20SIDS%20victims,cortical%20maturation%20compared%20to%20females.